[Table of Contents]


[Date Prev][Date Next][Thread Prev][Thread Next][Date Index][Thread Index]

[ARSCLIST] ABX for Dummies



I joined this list only recently, and was rather nonplussed that some early comments resulted in challenges to submit my personal observations to ABX testing. I demurred with as much good humor as I could muster, even after a suggestion that I was “afraid” to do so. Several references to Boston Audio Society papers were made, and I have noticed that other posters have gotten responses that reference the same test protocol, and that more posts have recently commented on it. While this list is far more civil than a Rec Audio newsgroup back when, it was evident that proposing an ABX test remains a convenient rhetorical device to dismiss an opinion with which one disagrees.

My undergraduate degree was in (Experimental) Psychology, so I have some conceptual understanding of and respect for the problems of test design. I was also a dues-paying member of the BAS back in the day (so was CJ, I think), so I am familiar with their fascination with ABX testing. My most recent encounter with such a test was at The Home Entertainment (parasite) Show at CES in Las Vegas last month. The tests were run by, believe it or not, the Wall Street Journal. The sound from either A/B choice was so a-musical I walked out, chuckling in amusement. But it was probably not the worst sound on hand in Vegas that week, either.

First, let me suggest that anyone who demands an ABX test as “proof” of another’s personal experience, but who has never actually designed or even participated in one is simply blowing smoke, and should refrain from doing so here.

Second, for anyone on the lists unfamiliar with ABX tests but given to intellectual curiosity, I can suggest a simple, free illustration of the process. ABX testing involves exposing a subject to sample A, then sample B. Sample X is then provided, and the subject decides whether it is identical to either A or B. If it is a double blind test, neither the subject nor the tester knows what A, B, or any X is. If you Google ABX, you will find a lot of angry audiophile bickering, but an ABX test can be designed for any situation that involves the cognitive processes of perception, memory, and identification. So, with apologies for the added bandwidth, a challenge follows:

Go to a paint store. As we want to avoid “coloration”, go to the rack of “Neutral” paint patch strips, and choose one where the differences in tint, hue, and saturation are minimal. (Of course, to be truly ”blind”, you can have someone else do all this). While the clerk is distracted, swipe 11 strips. After you get past mall security, pick the two patches that are the closest. Let’s say they are called Pewter Fog and Pearl Mist. Cut out the patches (eliminating the names) and label the backs of Pewter Fog A and the Pearl Mists B. Cover 10 sets of A and B labels with opaque but removable tape. Set aside the remaining pair labeled A and B, and dump the rest into a bowl deep enough that the contents cannot be seen or counted.

Place the two labeled A/B patches in front of the subject, label side up. Place one empty deep bowl labeled A to the left, and another labeled B to the right. Put the bowl of taped samples where they can be reached but not seen.

The subject (it can be yourself) can turn over A and examine it but must replace it face down to look at B, and vice-versa. Placing both face down, a sample is drawn from the bowl, and the subject must decide if it is identical to A or B, and place it in the appropriate bowl. The process is continued until all the samples are sorted. You can then peel back the tape and see how many were correctly ID’ed and sorted. Any more than 10 correct and you are better than chance. Score 20 and you can be referred to on this list as “Golden-Eyed”.

While this seems a pleasant enough parlor game, it would not cut the mustard in even an intro course in Experimental Psych. The result above is just a single data point; one needs another variable for the test to have meaning. Repeat the test several times to see if the skill can be learned. Test 100 people and sort by age, gender, religion, etc. Change the lighting from incandescent to fluorescent. Compare putative experts (artists, house painters) to the overall population. Choose different patches and correlate to the pigment differences. Etc. And of course, you can have lots of fun twisting the statistics, but we won’t go there right now.

If very few subjects can reliably tell the difference between Pearl Mist and Pewter Fog, they are perceptually identical, according to the statistics typically applied. If the new CEO of Benjamin Moore was hired from Crayola, he may decide to "dumb down" the variety of colors available and eliminate one (or both) colors from the line in order to impress the stockholders with his bold vision. Of course, the test actually tells us nothing about the qualities of paint that determine consumer preference – like durability, fade resistance, hiding power, etc. Or which shade your mother would choose for the dining room. And the marketing dept would never let such decisions be made “blind”, anyway. The use of evocative names acknowledges that some buyers would prefer a rich, luminous shade like Pearl Mist rather than a cool metallic one like Pewter Fog, even if the two shades can’t be distinguished by ABX testing. And of course, the putative “Golden-Eyes” like artists or housepainters would do no better than average folk because their background involves no special training in remembering subtleties of dried paint chips, even if they splash around in wet paint every day.

I realize of course, that resorting to “analogies” might rile up some of the more “scientific” types on the list, but perhaps actually participating in a simple double blind test can illuminate some of the grey (or is it gray?) areas in such discussions.

More to come.

Bruce


[Subject index] [Index for current month] [Table of Contents]